Saturday, January 26, 2013

The Demise of “Instinct” and Mana-tized Truths

            I'll start out by saying that this was written for a Contemporary Theory class that I am currently taking. It was bound to get out of hand with James, Freud, Adorno Horkheimer, and Said on the list, but it should have kernels of interest and contempt for all! I hope you enjoy! Of course, if you have any comments or critiques (including spiteful ones), I'll take 'em in the comments section or by email at bkramer.ru@gmail.com.

            Lacking a coherent place to start, I will dive into this entry with the help of an author that was not on the required reading list. Gaston Bachelard connects quite well with this week’s readings. He was mentioned overtly by Bourdieu last week, was a contemporary critic of both Freud and James, and was an influence on the Frankfurt School (i.e. Horkheimer and Adorno), Foucault (who Said draws from quite heavily), Derrida and Althusser. His concept of the “epistemological break” is explicit in each of these entries, apart from Freud. He states that “scientific progress always reveals a break, or better perceptual breaks, between ordinary knowledge and scientific knowledge” (Bachelard 1949: 270).  Affectively, he is challenging us to see through the doxa, and understand that we are approaching our own inquiries with a pre-established frame of reference, that in some cases, muddles our interpretations beyond repair. He is careful, like Bourdieu, not to take a nihilistic jump, but says rather that science is indeed useful for our understanding of the social and natural world.
            William James approached this in a new way around the turn of the century. I can sympathize with James’ notion of pragmatism, which is built on the implications of practice in any epistemological realm. He does well in turning away “from bad a priori reasons, from fixed principles, closed systems, and pretended absolutes and origins” (Kivisto 2008: 169). With this method, we are then free to use various theoretical tools to aid our understanding of the world. Like Nietzsche, he mentions the Almighty and his control over Euclidean space. Yet, as Nietzsche says “God is dead” (Kivisto 2008: 160), and thus so too is an a priori Euclidean understanding of space if we implement a pragmatic approach. Bachelard also took this prospect up with great diligence in his work on time-consciousness. Both show that by undermining some of these assumptions we can make progress in scientific understanding, among other things. Our new opinions proffer these epistemological breaks in thought, and from there we can construct a new concept of the world; with innovation and the openness of scientific experimentation.
Yet, where James lost me is when he asserts that “to a certain degree… everything here is plastic” (Kivisto 2008: 171). Our construction of a knowledge base becomes slippery and specious. Bachelard called this the “easy tolerance of the Pragmatists” (Jones 1991: 49), and is unfortunately a rather pervasive way of thinking; whether it be in the Thomas Theorem’s famous princess example or the apparent belief system of the Tea Party to ignore events that “actually” happened. In the right hands, this method can benefit us greatly, but it can also be a crux for the lazy scientist. But who does this right? Where do we start? Nietzsche forbade us from starting on theistic grounds, Said undermines the West-centric perspective, and Horkheimer and Adorno deem the tenets of the Enlightenment “totalitarian” as well. It seems we need to go back to the hegemonic forces that shape those frameworks and assumptions that we have in the first place, and as Bourdieu said, take up a barrage of arms to better conceive of our social milieu.
Freud offers some intriguing tools to help us interpret both psychological and macro-social phenomena.[1] Freud’s conception of drives is exemplar as an observation of closed systems (more on this later), as are the heuristic applications of the id, ego, and superego in clinical settings. I found his work on dreams intriguing, and, according to this clinical psychologist, is an applicable tool used toward understanding anxieties that are displaced in obsessive-compulsive tendencies (e.g. like checking fire alarms). This attests to the section about the repressed being manifested through the displacement of fear/anxiety in other real world objects (e.g. in hoarders). I would even agree with Freud’s approach to the oedipal complex, though only to a degree. I have my own reservations about the libidinal aspect of his theory, preferring to conceive of Eros and Thanatos more broadly, as propulsionary factors of closed systems in the physical world. It’s tough to say how much of this was implemented after Freud’s theory became popular, but the application of the oedipal complex is ubiquitous (see TLC’s Strange Sex Season 1, Episode 1 for an example of young men pursuing “cougars” in New York City). The “O.C,” more generally taken to examine conflict between cross-sex relationships with parents, has also been useful in dealing with friends and family in the past. Unfortunately, the defense mechanisms he lays out are all too real, especially when trying to suggest the prospect of change to those unwilling to listen. Interestingly, Willer (in press) uncovered some of these defense mechanisms experimentally in the case of the hyper-masculine.
However, my main discontent with Freud’s theory is his treatment of “instincts,” which is ultimately a lack of foresight in human ontogeny and of neural systems in particular.[2] This may not be pertinent to others (a necessary prerequisite to a tangent if you’ve ever read one), but I feel the application of his theory is a principle reason for the “nature vs. nurture” dichotomy and, consequently, the misconceptions that much of the public has regarding biology, sex/gender, and sexuality (Blumberg 2009; Foucault 1974). In other words, this, to me, demonstrates his hegemony over biological and genetic knowledge, even to the careful scientist without a background in those fields. In the article, he confounds “instincts,” “at birth,” and “innate.” To say these three things with the same intention is erroneous and has left scientists bickering for over a hundred years. We as “social scientists” don’t seem to care that cells exist prior to birth, because there is no behavior to observe and, thus, no “socialization.” For instance, Bergesen (2004) is perpetually sighted in cultural sociology texts, attesting to the pervasive imperialism that nativist-based evolutionary psychology, and best-selling authors like Elizabeth Spelke and Steven Pinker have over our field and the public at large. This happens even with suggestive evidence that they are the same sloppy scientists we condemn for wearing disciplinary blinders (see Spencer et al. 2009). Freud’s, and now our own, lack of understanding of how biological processes unfold in any environment (which is the social, provided there is a source of action (Latour 1997)), perpetuates the idea that we become objects that are overly-determined by miniscule genetic puppeteers. With this said, I fail to see how Freud’s conception of “innate drives” turns into anything different than a shadowy determinism based on the same mana, which he seemed so profoundly opposed to. We can circumvent these problems if we pick from a larger, denser “tree of knowledge” in order to better understand ontogeny and phylogeny (Maturana and Varela 1981). I believe this is what Bourdieu meant by arming ourselves with the weapons of a cross-disciplinary discourse to chat with our colleagues in the natural sciences. This is assuming again that the fruit we pick fills us with self-reflexivity, rather than vacuous presuppositions.  Of course, I am likely preaching to the choir here, since, for instance, biology is absolutely imperative in helping us strengthen our understanding of sex/gender. This is one example of an ongoing struggle that has been pervasive in science.
I was captivated by Said’s piece on Orientalism, and how he spoke of the Occidental framing the Orient. Sticking with science, I was brought back to the History of Science and Technology of Islam in Istanbul. Having visited a couple years ago, I remember how suspect I felt regarding many of the exhibits. I was brainwashed by my own education in the American empire of science, and I didn’t realize at the time how much innovation was stolen. This was especially pertinent since I was living in Córdoba, Spain; one of the most permeable centers of exchange between the “Occidental” and “Orient” up to la Reconquista. The heteronomy of the Roman-Catholic church is still strong there today, as I remember reading that three Muslims were arrested on charges of public disorder for praying in the now-dubbed “Catedral” in the city’s center. To a degree, I wonder if I know anything about the city now that its history seems so clearly Occidentalized to me. In the end, I suppose this suggests caution on all of our parts. Just as Freud’s “mana-tized” theory of instincts eventually monetized the American fetishism of consumption (i.e. Century of the Self) and Nietzsche’s “will to power” was strategically used to ignite the rise of struggling dictator in the Weimar Republic (Shirer 1959), we must all be reminded that our work will someday be seen as the hegemonic dogma of a generation pushing forth toward “progress,” “truth,” and “enlightenment.”

Bachelard, G. 1949. Le Rationalisme Appliqué. Presses Universitaires de France: Paris.

Bergesen, A. J. 2004. “Durkheim’s Theory of Mental Categories: A Review of the Evidence.” Annual Review of Sociology. 30: 395-408.

Blumberg, M. 2009. Freaks of Nature. Oxford University Press.

Bourdieu, Pierre. 1991. “On the Possibility of a Field of World Sociology.” Pp. 373-386 in Social Theory for a Changing Society, edited by Pierre Bourdieu and James Coleman.

Foucault, P. 1978. The History of Sexuality. Volume 1: An Introduction. Random House: New York, NY.

Horkheimer, Max & Theodor Adorno. 1944.  Dialectic of Enlightenment. Stanford University Press.

Jones, M. 1991. Gaston Bachelard, Subversive Humanist. University of Wisconsin Press. 

Kivisto, Peter (ed.) 2008. Social Theory: Roots and Branches, Third Edition: Oxford.

Maturana, H.R. & Varela, F.J. 1987. The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding. Shambhala: Boston, MA.

Said, Edward. 1978. Orientalism. Vintage Books.

Shirer, W.L. 1959. The Rise and Fall of the Third Reich, A History of Nazi Germany. Touchstone Publishing.

Spencer, John P., Mark S. Blumberg, Bob McMurray, Scott R. Robinson, Larissa K.
Samuelson, and J. Bruce Tomblin. 2009. “Short arms and talking eggs: Why we should no longer abide the nativist-empiricist debate.” Child Dev Perspect. August 1; 3 (2): 79–87.

Willer, Robb, Christabel Rogalin, Bridget Conlon, and Michael T. Wojnowicz. 2013. "Overdoing Gender: A Test of the Masculine Overcompensation Thesis.American Journal of Sociology. In press.




[1] My inclinations toward Freud have always been negative, though this bias has recently been alleviated by an ongoing discourse with a clinical psychologist.
[2] Of course, he did better than Bergson or Mead as psycho-physiologists, or their attempts thereof.

No comments:

Post a Comment